|
|||||
![]() |
|||||
![]() |
Inconsistency between concept and measurement: The Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI) Elena Svetieva & Michael Walker, School of Psychology, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia. E-mail: elenas@psych.usyd.edu.auAbstract "Problem" and "pathological" gambling represent core concepts that guide gambling research today. However, divergent interpretation of the relation between these terms is continually misguiding the measurement and interpretation of empirical data, and may cumulatively lead to larger-scale problems of conclusion and policy formulation over the next decade. This paper first attempts to unravel the conceptual muddle by outlining the trajectory of the usage of the two terms, from a period where both were dimensionally similar concepts firmly situated in the addiction model to a more recent conception, which takes the view that problem gambling is distinct and properly measured by focusing on the problems that excessive gambling may cause to individuals, families, and communities. We then aim to analyse and criticize the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI) as a clear example of the confusion of paradigms, an index that defines problem gambling in the newer, problem-centred model, but continues to measure it with items reflecting the older, addiction-centred model. We argue that results obtained using the CPGI, much like those of its predecessors, will not adequately capture the notion of harm that underpins current definitions of problem gambling. Keywords: problem gambling, pathological gambling, measurement, definition, CPGI
Introduction Research in problem gambling is notorious for being plagued with a multitude of terms that seek to capture the construct, including "compulsive," "pathological," and "problem" gambling. These terms are ill-defined, often being used interchangeably and without an understanding of their theoretical origins and associated paradigms. Over the years, pragmatic concerns have relegated conceptual distinction to the wayside, with the view that making advances in treatment efficacy is more important than what is seen as a largely academic debate over terms and concepts. However, we wish to draw attention to an important conceptual distinction concerning the definition of problem gambling and to show how, in certain jurisdictions, failure to understand this conceptual distinction has led to muddled thinking, resulting in a bifurcation between concept and practice in the measurement of problem gambling, a bifurcation that may have significant consequences for future research and policy decisions.
The addiction-based concept of problem gambling In the literature on problem gambling, there are two quite different conceptions of what problem gambling is. The earlier conception has its origins in the development of the related concept of pathological gambling. Pathological gambling was added to the list of psychiatric disorders in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980). In the revision of this manual published in 1987 (DSM-III-R), the criteria for pathological gambling were based on those for substance dependence, and an underlying explanatory model of addiction was assumed (Walker, 1992). At the same time, the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS; Lesieur & Blume, 1987) was developed as a clinical screen for diagnosing individuals as probable pathological gamblers. Importantly, the SOGS included items concerned with preoccupation, tolerance, withdrawal, and loss of control that overlapped with the DSM-III-R and are the core constructs of an addiction model. Thus, the construct of pathological gambling is not theoretically neutral either in its definition or in its measurement. Rather, this construct assumes that gambling can become an addiction that can be clinically diagnosed by signs that are similar to other addictions. Indeed, excessive gambling has been referred to as a "pure addiction" because the addiction exists without the associated chemical component of drug addiction (Custer & Milt, 1985; Jacobs, 1986). In the SOGS, probable pathological gambling is diagnosed by a score of five or more positive answers to the 20 items on the list. Problem gambling is defined as a score of three or four positive answers to the same list of 20 items. Thus, the concept of problem gambling inherent in the SOGS is as a weaker form of pathological gambling. Pathological gambling and problem gambling share a common theoretical basis as terms related to addiction to gambling. There is now an extensive literature of research and argument that assumes that problem gambling and pathological gambling are closely related and only differ in severity. This conception of problem gambling as a weaker form of pathological gambling is explicitly stated in research on the prevalence of pathological and problem gambling (Petry & Tawfik, 2001). Problem and pathological gambling lie on the same dimension and differ only in the severity of the addiction. We label this older view "addiction-based" to discriminate it from the more recent "problem-centred" view.
The problem-centred concept of problem gambling The alternative problem-centred view of problem gambling is becoming increasingly popular. This more recent conceptualization is based in the distinction between excessive gambling behaviour and problems that are a consequence of that behaviour (Walker, 1992). By defining problem gambling as gambling problems that result from excessive gambling behaviour, this alternative conceptualization of problem gambling remains theoretically neutral. In particular, there is no implication that problem gambling involves an addiction to gambling. The focus of this alternative view of problem gambling is the degree of harm caused to the individual, not the mechanisms by which the gambling behaviour becomes excessive. It is a viewpoint that has found widespread international acceptance. In the United States (Cox, Lesieur, Rosenthal & Volberg, 1997; Lesieur, 1998), Canada (Ferris & Wynne, 2001), and Britain (Sproston, Erens, & Orford, 2000), problem gambling has been defined to encompass all gambling behaviour types and patterns that cause disruption and damage to a person’s functioning. Similarly in Australia, Dickerson, McMillen, Hallebone, Volberg, and Woolley (1997) defined problem gambling as "the situation when a person’s gambling activity gives rise to harm to the individual player, and/or to his family, and may extend into the community" (p. 106). By highlighting the harms caused by excessive gambling behaviour, this alternative definition of problem gambling fits well within a public health model. The public health approach is a broad framework providing the perspective that problem gambling "is not restricted to a narrow focus on gambling addiction" (Shaffer, 2003, p. 15); that is, it is not just a problem of addiction and individual psychopathology, but rather a problem that exists in a social setting, is multiply determined, and has broad community effects (Korn, Gibbins, & Azmier, 2003; Shaffer, 2003). Rather than focusing on the addiction-like attributes of excessive gambling, the public health model focuses on harm caused by gambling, and by this virtue is designed to allow a better determination of the socio-economic impacts of gambling. This model also has several policy functions. Gamblers experiencing harm may not necessarily be those experiencing severe personal or psychological problems. By limiting the count of problem gamblers to those with specific psychological or psychiatric symptom profiles, policy efforts may fail to reach the larger numbers of individuals who are harmed by excessive gambling. Furthermore, as Shaffer and Korn (2002) point out, although members of this larger group may not be suffering from severe psychological impairment or psychopathology at the individual level, they collectively have the greatest impact on the community. For this reason, greater individual and community benefit may accumulate from intervention, treatment, and education measures directed at this larger group that is defined by the range and intensity of the harms caused by gambling.
In this view, gambling prevalence research is similar to psychiatric epidemiology that directs treatment, harm reduction, and prevention efforts. This view also makes the assumption that problem gambling is a psychiatric disorder, that there exists a point at which gambling becomes "unhealthy," and that problem gambling is a diagnosable "illness." It is beyond the scope of this paper to review the empirical evidence that problem gambling is an illness or psychopathology (see Walker, 1992; Walker & Dickerson, 1996), or to restate any view that the illness model serves a socio-political rather than a scientific function (see Rosecrance, 1985). Suffice to say, the assumption is contentious and places the public health model into a theory-laden framework. The greater risk is that this assumption may further legitimize the use of clinical screening tools in prevalence studies, while at the same time ignoring the true implications of theory-neutral and widely accepted public-health definitions of problem gambling.
Implications of the problem-centred concept for measurement The problem-centred view of problem gambling assumes that excessive gambling behaviour causes a range of problems for the individual, for his or her family, and for the community. What constitutes excessive gambling depends on the characteristics of the individual and the extent to which his or her circumstances will tolerate a greater or lesser expenditure of money and time on the activity. It is not simply the volume of gambling or the size of the loss that defines problem gambling. There may be heavy time and monetary expenditure on gambling activities, but if there are no consequences of that behaviour, as may often be the case for gamblers with ample leisure time and large disposable incomes, then this cannot constitute problem gambling in the public health sense. It follows that it is not the characteristics of the gambling itself that define problem gambling but rather the fact that an individual may not limit the expenditure of money and time to stay within reasonable bounds of the resources available. The characteristics of the gambling may include the attributes of addiction, but this aspect alone is not a necessary or sufficient condition for the presence of problem gambling. A person may exhibit a preoccupation with gambling, tolerance effects for session length, and withdrawal symptoms when gambling ceases. However, if the gambling involves sufficiently small bets, no problems may follow. Chasing losses is frequently associated with problem gambling, but it is not the act of chasing that marks the onset of problem gambling, but the fact that chasing may cause the individual to cross the critical threshold of tolerable monetary loss. It is for this reason that we have seen an emergence of harm-based conceptualization of problem gambling in the past decade (Dickerson et al., 1997; Neal, Delfabbro, & O’Neil, 2005). For example, Neal et al. (2005) state, "Problem gambling is characterized by difficulties in limiting money and/or time spent on gambling which leads to adverse consequences for the gambler, others, or for the community" (p. 125). According to these widely accepted definitions, if the expenditure of money and time do not cause problems for the individual, others, or the community, then the gambling does not meet the necessary condition for the occurrence of problem gambling. It follows that problem gambling must be measured by the number and extent of the problems caused by the gambling, not by whether or not the gambling behaviour has the characteristics of addiction or any other individual psychopathology. The re-conceptualization of problem gambling in terms of the harm caused by excessive gambling implies a re-evaluation of the methods by which problem gambling is screened and measured. The SOGS (Lesieur & Blume, 1987) has been used in almost all problem gambling prevalence research across the United States, Asia, Europe, and Canada (Shaffer, Hall & Vander Bilt, 1999; Sproston et al., 2000; Volberg, Abbott, Ronnberg, & Munck, 2001). Although recent prevalence studies in the United States (Gerstein et al., 1999) have also used a screen based on DSM-IV (1994), such as the NODS, the SOGS remains as one of the most widely used prevalence measures in the world (Abbot & Volberg, 2006). Over the period of its use, the SOGS has received an accumulation of criticism directed at the context and assumptions behind its development (Volberg, 2001), its outdated criteria (Volberg, 1996), and the validity of its estimates (Walker & Dickerson, 1996).
The Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI) One recently developed scale, the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI), has received attention as a potential successor to previous instruments. Developed largely as a response to the criticism around the SOGS, the CPGI has been presented as a modern and promising tool for use in problem gambling prevalence research. The scale as a whole contains 31 items (plus demographics) that cover gambling involvement, problem gambling assessment, and correlates of problem gambling (Ferris & Wynne, 2001). Only nine of those items are scored, and they comprise the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI), an index designed to serve both as a prevalence measure and a general population screen that is brief, reliable, and provides adequate estimates of the problem. In the first stage of the development of the CPGI, the Canadian Inter-Provincial Task Force on Problem Gambling adopted the following definition of problem gambling: "Problem gambling is gambling behaviour that creates negative consequences for the gambler, others in his or her social network, or for the community" (Ferris & Wynne, 2001, Introduction at 1.2). This definition takes as its focus the consequences or harm of gambling activity, and is very similar to that proposed earlier by Dickerson et al. (1997) and Neal et al. (2005). Although it is a problem-centred definition suitable for use within the public health model, the developers of the CPGI state that that they still sought to develop the PGSI as a measure of both problem behaviour and adverse consequences (Ferris & Wynne, 2001). In addition to adopting a harm-based operational definition, the PGSI also involved the creation of a range of categories into which respondents may fall: non-gamblers, non-problem gamblers, low-risk gamblers, moderate-risk gamblers, and problem gamblers. The ordinal sub-types of the PGSI suggest a problem gambling continuum, and so are seen as a substantial improvement to the dichotomous and discrete variables encompassed in instruments such as the DSM-IV (1994) and the SOGS. In brief, in the CPGI’s rationale and associated features, the developers of the CPGI claim that it is "a new, more meaningful measure of problem gambling for use in general adult population surveys, one that reflect[s] a more holistic view of gambling, and include[s] more indicators of social context" (Ferris & Wynne, 2001, p. 1.1).
An analysis of the CPGI The development of the CPGI was associated with an explicit rejection of a medicalized model of pathological gambling in favour of a view of problem gambling as a social issue with public health consequences (Ferris & Wynne, 2001). However, in the light of such a framework and the explicit model adopted, it is important to analyse the actual items chosen for the PGSI. Table 1 The first, and most problematic aspect of the PGSI, is that its items were drawn from scales measuring pathological gambling. The PGSI uses items largely borrowed from instruments (SOGS and DSM-IV) that have been developed in a U.S. clinical context and for the express purpose of identifying pathological gambling. Using a content analysis of the items in the PGSI and SOGS, McMillen and Wenzel (2006) concluded that the PGSI and the SOGS show considerable overlap in their content, much more so than their foundational differences may suggest. Furthermore, the PGSI includes an item taken from the DSM-IV related to tolerance (Item 2), a core construct of the addiction model. The authors claim that the inclusion was justified because "DSM-IV items ... capture the more severe end of the gambling problem spectrum very well" (Ferris & Wynne, 2001, Results section 3.2). By adopting the older concept of problem gambling as a less severe form of pathological gambling, this statement undermines the explicit claims that the CPGI concept of problem gambling derives from the public health model.
Validation of the CPGI The validation of any prevalence measure necessitates a different process. As Thomas, Jackson, and Blaszczynski (2003) note:
Thus, either an index is designed to reflect a theory about problem gambling and then evaluated using statistical procedures, or, if an index was compiled from previous instruments using statistical procedures, then it should be evaluated on its theoretical coherence.
This indirect approach is symptomatic both of the fact that no objective external criteria were available to assess validity and of the study’s general failure to directly consider the question of what is problem gambling when evaluating the items. The problem is made even more difficult when the DSM-IV and SOGS items are clearly founded in an addiction model of gambling, making it difficult to specify what measures would indeed be appropriate and independent.
Requirement for an adequate measure of gambling-related harm There is a second major cluster of problems that surround the CPGI, in that the items in the PGSI that supposedly measure harm are not comprehensive. The promise of the CPGI lies in the harm-based definition adopted in Phase I, as well as in the emerging Canadian view that problem gambling is a public health issue. However, the actual PGSI items that are claimed to measure harm are questionable in this capacity. These consist of Items 6-9 (Ferris & Wynne, 2001, see Table 1).
Measuring the problem-centred concept of problem gambling The public health definition of problem gambling (as excessive gambling behaviour that causes harm to the individual at personal and interpersonal levels) implies an approach to measurement that is independent of previous work, where the addiction model of gambling was an inherent part of the definition of problem gambling. Given the reliance of the CPGI on items drawn from addiction-based measures of pathological gambling, and given the criticisms of the actual measurements made by the CPGI, it would seem important to regard this instrument as not measuring problem gambling as defined in the public health context. It is the view of the authors that the continued use of the CPGI in research on the prevalence of problem gambling will cause errors in the interpretation of the severity of problem gambling throughout the world, errors in understanding the relation of problem gambling to its causes and distribution throughout communities, and errors in deciding best policy and practice in the attempts of governments to deal with this major social problem. What is required is an independent approach to measurement that does not rely on prior research conducted within the addiction framework. The public health definition implies that both gambling behaviour and the harms caused by that behaviour must be measured. Thus, a dual index would seem necessary. Public health policy must focus on the harm caused to individuals by the presence and availability of widespread gambling opportunities. Of all the measurement instruments that must be developed to appropriately monitor problem gambling, the one that appears central is that which measures the cumulative harm to the individual across the major domains of human functioning. A statistically sound measure of harm caused would seem a necessary next step in problem gambling research. Some attempts to develop such an index already exist, including the gambling addiction severity index (Lesieur & Blume, 1990; Petry, 2003), the HARM scale (Productivity Commission, 1999), and the gambling treatment outcome monitoring system (GAMTOMS; Stinchfield, Winters, Botzet, Jerstad, & Breyer, 2007). Each one aims to assess gambling-related problems in all the main domains of functioning, including the financial, legal, occupational, and psychosocial domain, giving a pure, comprehensive, and composite measure of gambling-related harm. Although none are without major criticism, these earlier attempts give some guidance to the kind of reasoning needed to measure harm caused by gambling successfully. The way forward may well begin with the logic behind these instruments.
References Abbott, M.W., & Volberg, R.A. (2006). The measurement of adult problem and pathological gambling. International Gambling Studies, 6 (2), 175-200. Allcock, C. (1995, November). Some ponderings on pathological gambling: An introspective essay. Paper presented at the Sixth National Conference of the National Association for Gambling Studies, Sydney, Australia. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Battersby, M., Thomas, L.J., Tolchard, B., & Esterman, A. (2002). The South Oaks Gambling Screen: A review with reference to Australian use. Journal of Gambling Studies, 18 (3), 257-271. Cosgrave, J., & Klassen, T.R. (2001). Gambling against the state: The state and the legitimation of gambling. Current Sociology, 49 (5), 1-15. Cox, S., Lesieur, H.R., Rosenthal, R.J., & Volberg, R.A. (1997). Problem and pathological gambling in America: The national picture. Columbia, MD: National Council on Problem Gambling. Custer, R.L., & Milt, H. (1985). When luck runs out. New York: Facts on File Publications. Dickerson, M.G., Baron, E., Hong, S.M., & Cottroll, D. (1996). Estimating the extent and degree of gambling related problems in the Australian population: A national survey. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12, 161-178. Dickerson M., McMillen J., Hallebone E., Volberg R., & Woolley R. (1997). Definition and incidence of problem gambling, including the socio-economic distribution of problem gamblers. Melbourne, Victoria, Australia: Victorian Casino and Gaming Authority. Eadington, W.R. (1998). Contributions of casino-style gambling to local economies. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 556, 53-65. Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final report. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. Gerstein, D., Murphy, S., Toce, M., Hoffmann, J., Palmer, A., Johnson, R., et al. (1999). Gambling Impact and Behavior Study. Report to the National Gambling Impact Study Commission. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago, National Opinion Research Center. Govoni, R., Frisch, G.R., & Stinchfield, R. (2001). A critical review of screening and assessment instruments for problem gambling. Windsor, ON: University of Windsor Problem Gambling Research Group. Retrieved from http://www.gamblingresearch.org/download.sz/Critical_Review.pdf?docid=4120 Gusfield, J.R. (1967). Moral passage: The symbolic process in public designations of deviance. Social Problems, 15 (2), 175-188. Jacobs, D.F. (1986). A general theory of addictions: A new theoretical model. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 15-31. Korn, D.A., Gibbins, R., & Azmier, J. (2003). Framing public policy towards a public health paradigm for gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19 (2), 235-256. Ladouceur R., Jacques C., Chevalier S., Sevigny S., & Hamel D. (2005). Prevalence of pathological gambling in Quebec in 2002. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 50 (8), 451-456. Lesieur, H. (1998). Costs and treatment of pathological gambling. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 556, 153-171. Lesieur, H., & Blume, S.B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1184-1188. Lesieur, H., & Blume, S.B. (1990). Modifying the Addiction Severity Index for use with pathological gamblers. Amityville, NY: South Oaks Institute. McCready, J., & Adlaf, E. (2006). Performance and enhancement of the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI): Report and recommendations. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. McMillen, J., Marshall, D., Ahmed, E., & Wenzel, M. (2004). 2003 Victorian Longitudinal Community Attitudes Survey. Canberra, Australia: The Centre for Gambling Research, Australian National University. McMillen, J., & Wenzel, M. (2006). Measuring problem gambling: Assessment of three prevalence screens. International Gambling Studies, 6, 147-174. Neal, P., Delfabbro, P., & O’Neil, M. (2005). Problem gambling and harm: A national definition. Adelaide, Australia: South Australian Centre for Economic Studies. O’Hara, J. (1998). A mug’s game: A history of gambling and betting in Australia. New South Wales, Australia: University Press. Petry, N.M. (2003). Validity of a gambling scale for the Addiction Severity Index. Journal of Nervous Mental Disease, 191 (6), 399-407. Petry, N.M., & Tawfik, Z. (2001). A comparison of problem gambling and non-problem gambling youth seeking treatment for marijuana abuse. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1324–1331. Productivity Commission. (1999). Australia’s gambling industries (Report No. 10). Canberra, Australia: Author. Queensland Treasury. (2001). Queensland Household Gambling Survey 2001. Brisbane, Australia: Queensland Government. Queensland Treasury. (2005). Queensland Household Gambling Survey 2003-04. Brisbane, Australia: Queensland Government. Rosecrance, J. (1985). Compulsive gambling and the medicalization of deviance. Social Roy Morgan Research. (2006). The fourth study into the extent and impact of gambling in Tasmania with particular reference to problem gambling: Follow-up to the studies conducted in 1994, 1996 and 2000. Hobart, Tasmania: Gambling Support Bureau, Human Services and Housing Division, Department of Health and Services. Shaffer, H.J. (2003). A public health perspective on gambling: The four principles. AGA Responsible Gaming Lecture Series, 2 (1), 1-27. Shaffer, H., Hall, M., & Vander Bilt, J. (1999). Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States: A research synthesis. American Journal of Public Health, 89 (9), 1369-1376. Shaffer, H., & Korn, D. (2002). Gambling and related mental disorders: A public health analysis. Annual Review of Public Health, 23, 171–212. Sproston, K., Erens, B., & Orford, J. (2000). Gambling behaviour in Britain: Results from the British Gambling Prevalence Survey. London: The National Centre for Social Research. Stinchfield, R. (2002). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS). Addictive Behaviours, 27 (1), 1-19. Stinchfield, R., Winters, K.C., Botzet, A., Jerstad, S., & Breyer, J. (2007) Development and psychometric evaluation of the gambling treatment outcome monitoring system (GAMTOMS). Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21 (2), 174-184. Strong, D.R., Breen, R.B., & Lejuez, C.W. (2004). Using item response theory to examine gambling attitudes and beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1515–1529. Thomas, S., Jackson, A., & Blaszczynski, A. (2003). Measuring problem gambling: Evaluation of the Victorian Gambling Screen (Gambling Research Panel Report No. 4). Melbourne, Victoria, Australia: Melbourne Enterprise International. Volberg, R.A. (1996). Prevalence studies of problem gambling in the United States. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12 (2), 111–128. Volberg, R.A. (2001). When the chips are down: Problem gambling in America. New York: Century Foundation. Volberg, R.A., Abbott, M.W., Ronnberg, S., & Munck, I.M. (2001). Prevalence and risks of pathological gambling in Sweden. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104 (4), 250-256. Volberg, R.A., & Bernhard, B. (2006). The 2006 study of gambling and problem gambling in New Mexico. Report to the Responsible Gaming Association of New Mexico. Northampton, MA: Gemini Research. Walker, M. (1992). The psychology of gambling. Oxford, England: Butterworth-Heinemann. Walker, M.B., & Dickerson, M.G. (1996). The prevalence of problem gambling and pathological gambling: A critical analysis. Journal of Gambling Studies 12 (2), 233-249. Wardle, H, Sproston, K., Orford, J., Erens, B., Griffiths, M., Constantine, R., & Pigott, S. (2007). British Gambling Prevalence Survey 2007. National Centre for Social Research. London, United Kingdom. Wenzel, M., McMillen, J., Marshall, D., & Ahmed, E. (2004). Validation of the Victorian Gambling Screen. Melbourne, Victoria, Australia: Gambling Research Panel. Wynne, H. (2003). Introducing the Canadian Problem Gambling Index. Edmonton, AB: Wynne Resources. ******* Manuscript history: submitted November 25, 2007; accepted July 23, 2008. This article was peer-reviewed. All URLs were available at the time of submission. For correspondence: Elena Svetieva, University of Sydney, Gambling Treatment Clinic, School of Psychology, A18, University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW 2006, Australia, phone (+612) 9036 9336, elenas@psych.usyd.edu.au Contributors: Both authors have been personally and actively involved in substantive work leading to the report. Competing interests: None declared. Ethics approval: None required. Funding: ES and MW are employed at the Gambling Treatment Clinic (GTC), University of Sydney. The GTC is funded by the NSW Government’s Rehabilitation Gambling Fund. Professor Dr. Michael Walker is Associate Professor of Psychology, Director of the Gambling Treatment Clinic and Co-Director of the Gambling Research Unit at The University of Sydney. Elena Svetieva is a Research Associate at the Gambling Treatment Clinic. |
![]() |
|
issue 22 — December 2008 ![]() |
contents | submissions | links | archive | subscribe
Please note that these links will always point to the current issue of JGI. To navigate previous issues, use the sidebar links near the top of the page.
Copyright © 1999-2008 The Centre for Addiction and Mental Health
Editorial Contact:
Join our list to be notified of new issues.
Subscribe